Titulo:

Inhibitory processes and cognitive flexibility: evidence for the theory of attentional inertia
.

Sumario:

The aim of this study was to discriminate the differential contribution of different inhibitory processes -perceptual, cognitive and behavioral inhibition- to switching cost effect associated with alternation cognitive tasks. A correlational design was used. Several experimental paradigms (e.g., Stop signal, visual search, Stemberg´s experimental and Simon paradigm) were adapted and included in a computerized program called TAC (Introzzi & Canet Juric, 2014) to the assessment of the different cognitive processes. The final sample consisted of 45 adults (18-50 years). Perceptual and behavioral inhibition shows moderate and low correlations with attentional cost, cognitive inhibition shows no relation with flexibility and only per... Ver más

Guardado en:

2011-2084

2011-7922

8

2015-07-01

61

75

International Journal of Psychological Research - 2015

info:eu-repo/semantics/openAccess

http://purl.org/coar/access_right/c_abf2

id metarevistapublica_unisanbuenaventura_internationaljournalofpsychologicalresearch_21_article_1510
record_format ojs
institution UNIVERSIDAD DE SAN BUENAVENTURA
thumbnail https://nuevo.metarevistas.org/UNIVERSIDADDESANBUENAVENTURA_COLOMBIA/logo.png
country_str Colombia
collection International Journal of Psychological Research
title Inhibitory processes and cognitive flexibility: evidence for the theory of attentional inertia
spellingShingle Inhibitory processes and cognitive flexibility: evidence for the theory of attentional inertia
Introzzi, Isabel
Canet-Juric, Lorena
Montes, Silvana
López, Soledad
Mascarello, Graziella
Inhibitory process
cognitive flexibility
Attentional Inertia Theory.
title_short Inhibitory processes and cognitive flexibility: evidence for the theory of attentional inertia
title_full Inhibitory processes and cognitive flexibility: evidence for the theory of attentional inertia
title_fullStr Inhibitory processes and cognitive flexibility: evidence for the theory of attentional inertia
title_full_unstemmed Inhibitory processes and cognitive flexibility: evidence for the theory of attentional inertia
title_sort inhibitory processes and cognitive flexibility: evidence for the theory of attentional inertia
description The aim of this study was to discriminate the differential contribution of different inhibitory processes -perceptual, cognitive and behavioral inhibition- to switching cost effect associated with alternation cognitive tasks. A correlational design was used. Several experimental paradigms (e.g., Stop signal, visual search, Stemberg´s experimental and Simon paradigm) were adapted and included in a computerized program called TAC (Introzzi & Canet Juric, 2014) to the assessment of the different cognitive processes. The final sample consisted of 45 adults (18-50 years). Perceptual and behavioral inhibition shows moderate and low correlations with attentional cost, cognitive inhibition shows no relation with flexibility and only perceptual inhibition predicts switching costs effects, suggesting that different inhibitory processes contribute differentially to switch cost. This could be interpreted as evidence to Attentional Inertia Theory main argument which postulates that inhibition plays an essential role in the ability to flexibly switch between tasks and/or representations.
author Introzzi, Isabel
Canet-Juric, Lorena
Montes, Silvana
López, Soledad
Mascarello, Graziella
author_facet Introzzi, Isabel
Canet-Juric, Lorena
Montes, Silvana
López, Soledad
Mascarello, Graziella
topicspa_str_mv Inhibitory process
cognitive flexibility
Attentional Inertia Theory.
topic Inhibitory process
cognitive flexibility
Attentional Inertia Theory.
topic_facet Inhibitory process
cognitive flexibility
Attentional Inertia Theory.
citationvolume 8
citationissue 2
publisher Universidad San Buenaventura - USB (Colombia)
ispartofjournal International Journal of Psychological Research
source https://revistas.usb.edu.co/index.php/IJPR/article/view/1510
language spa
format Article
rights https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/
International Journal of Psychological Research - 2015
info:eu-repo/semantics/openAccess
http://purl.org/coar/access_right/c_abf2
references Allport, D. A., Styles, E. A., & Hsieh, S. (1994). Shifting intentional set: Exploring the dynamic control of tasks. In C. Umilta, & M. Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and performance XV (pp. 421-452). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Allport, A., & Wyllie, G. (1999). Task-switching: Positive and negative priming of task-set. In G. W. Humphreys, J. Duncan, & A. M. Treisman (Eds.), Attention, space an action: Studies in cognitive neuroscience (pp. 273-296). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Allport, A., & Wylie, G. (2000). Task switching, stimulus-response bindings, and negative priming. In S. Monsell & J. Driver (Eds.), Control of cognitive processes: Attention and performance XVIII (pp. 35–70). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Anderson, M. C., & Bjork, R. A. (1994). Mechanisms of inhibition in long-term memory: A new taxonomy. In D. Dagenbach & T. Carr (Eds.), Inhibitory processes in attention, memory, and language (pp. 265-325). New York: Academic Press. Altmann, E. M., & Gray, W. D. (2008). An integrated model of cognitive control in task switching. Psychological Review, 115(3), 602-639. Arbuthnott, K. D. (2005). The effect of repeated imagery on memory. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 19(7), 843-866. Band, G. P. H., van der Molen, M., & Logan, G. (2003). Horse-race model simulations of the stop-signal procedure, Acta Psychologica. 112, 105-142. Darowski, E. S., Helder, E., Zacks, R. T., Hasher, L., & Hambrick, D. Z. (2008). Age-Related Differences in Cognition: The Role of Distraction Control. Neuropsychology, 22(5), 638-644. Davidson, M. C., Amso, D., Anderson, L. C., & Diamond, A. (2006). Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4 to 13 years: Evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching. Neuropsychologia, 44, 2037-2078. R E S E A R C H INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH Procesos Inhibitorios y Flexibilidad Cognitiva Introzzi et al. (2015) Int.j.psychol.res. 8 (2) PP. 60 - 74 73 Diamond, A. (2013) Executive Functions. Annual Review of Psychology. 64, 135-168. Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception & Psychophysics, 16, 143-149. Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2004). The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: A latent-variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 133, 101-135. Gandolfi, E., Viterbori, P., Traverso, L., & Usai, M. C. (2014). Inhibitory processes in toddlers: a latent-variable approach. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1–11. Goschke, T. (2000). Decomposing the central executive: Persistence, deactivation, and reconfiguration of voluntary task set. In S. Monsell and J. Driver (Eds.), Control of cognitive processes: Attention and performance XVIII (pp. 331-356). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Introzzi, I. & Canet Juric, L. (2014). Evaluación de las Funciones Ejecutivas. XVIII Congreso Nacional de Psicodiagnóstico. Asociación Argentina de Estudio e Investigación en Psicodiagnóstico ADEIP. Mar del Plata, Argentina. Disponible en www.adeip.org.ar/Congreso2014 Jersild, A.T. (1927). Mental set and shift. Archives of Psychology, 89, 5-82. Joormann, J., & Gotlib, I.H. (2008). Updating the contents of working memory in depression: Interference from irrelevant negative material. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117, 206-213. Kirkham, N. Z., Cruess, L., & Diamond, A. (2003). Helping children apply their knowledge to their behavior on a dimension-switching task. Developmental Science, 6, 449-467. Kirkham, N. Z., & Diamond, A. (2003). Sorting between theories of perseveration: Performance in conflict tasks requires memory, attention, and inhibition. Developmental Science, 6, 474-476. Koch, I., Gade, M., Schuch, S., & Philipp, A. (2010). The role of inhibition in task switching: A review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17(1), 1-14. Logan, G. D., Schachar, R. J., & Tannock, R. (1997). Impulsivity and inhibitory control. Psychological Science, 8, 60-64. Mayr, U., & Keele, S. W. (2000). Changing internal constraints on action: The role of backward inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129, 4-26. Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. (2012). The Nature and Organization of individual differences in Executive Functions: Four General Conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(1), 8-14. Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M., Witzki, A. & Howerter, A. (2000). The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and their contributions to Complex "Frontal Lobe" Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49-100. Monsell, S. (2003). Task switching. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(3), 134-140. Morein-Zamir, S., & Kingstone, A. (2006) Fixation offset and stop signal intensity effects on saccadic countermanding: a crossmodal investigation. Experimental Brain Research, 175, 3453-462. Nigg, J. T. (2000). On inhibition/disinhibition in developmental psychopathology: Views from cognitive and personality psychology and a working inhibition taxonomy. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 220-246. Oberauer, K. (2001). Removing irrelevant information from working memory: Individual and age differences in short-term recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 27, 948–957. Oberauer K. (2002). Access to information in working memory: exploring the focus of attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition, 28, 411–421. Osman, A., Kornblum, S., & Meyer, D.E. (1986). The Point-of-No-Return in Choice Reaction-Time - Controlled and Ballistic Stages of Response Preparation. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 12, 243–258. Osman, A., Kornblum, S., & Meyer, D. E. (1990). Does response programming necessitate response execution? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 16, 183-198. Philipp, A. M., Kalinich, C., Koch, I., & Schubotz, R. I. (2008). Mixing costs and switch costs when switching stimulus dimensions in serial predictions. Psychological Research, 72, 405–414. Schachar, R. J., & Logan, G. D. (1990). Impulsivity and inhibitory control in normal development and childhood psychopathology. Developmental Psychology, 26, 710–720. R E S E A R C H INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH Procesos Inhibitorios y Flexibilidad Cognitiva Introzzi et al. (2015) Int.j.psychol.res. 8 (2) PP. 60 - 74 74 Schachar, R., Tannock, R., Marriott, M., & Logan, G. (1995). Deficient inhibitory control in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 23, 411– 437. Schneider, D. W., & Logan, G. D. (2005). Modeling task switching without switching tasks: A short-term priming account of explicitly cued performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134, 343-367. Simon, J. R., & Rudell, A, P. (1967). Auditory S-R compatibility: The effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. Journal of Applied Psychology, 51,300- 304. Sternberg, S. (1969). Memory-scanning: Mental processes revealed by reaction-time experiments. American Scientist, 57(4), 421-457. Tresiman, A.M., & Gelade, G. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97-136. Treisman, A., & Sato, S. (1990). Conjunction search revisited. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 16, 459-478. Tsal, Y., Shalev, L., & Mevorach, C. (2005). The diversity of attention deficits in ADHD: The prevalence of four cognitive factors in ADHD versus controls. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 38(2), 142-157. Verbruggen, F., & De Houwer, J. (2007). Do emotional stimuli interfere with response inhibition? Evidence from the stop signal paradigm. Cognition & Emotion, 21, 391-40. Verbruggen, F., & Logan, G. D. (2009). Automaticity of cognitive control: Goal priming in response-inhibition paradigms. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35(5), 1381-1388. Verbruggen, F., Logan, G.D., & Stevens, M.A. (2008). STOP--‐IT: Windows executable software for the stop-signal paradigm. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 479-483. Waszak, F., Hommel, B., & Allport, A. (2003). Task-switching and long-term priming: Role of episodic stimulus-task bindings in task-shift costs. Cognitive Psychology, 46, 361-413. Wylie, G., & Allport, A. (2000). Task switching and the measurement of “switch costs. Psychological Research, 63, 212-233 Yeung, N., & Monsell, S. (2003). Switching between tasks of unequal familiarity: The role of stimulus attribute and response-set selection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29(2), 455–469
type_driver info:eu-repo/semantics/article
type_coar http://purl.org/coar/resource_type/c_6501
type_version info:eu-repo/semantics/publishedVersion
type_coarversion http://purl.org/coar/version/c_970fb48d4fbd8a85
type_content Text
publishDate 2015-07-01
date_accessioned 2015-07-01T00:00:00Z
date_available 2015-07-01T00:00:00Z
url https://revistas.usb.edu.co/index.php/IJPR/article/view/1510
url_doi https://doi.org/10.21500/20112084.1510
issn 2011-2084
eissn 2011-7922
doi 10.21500/20112084.1510
citationstartpage 61
citationendpage 75
url2_str_mv https://revistas.usb.edu.co/index.php/IJPR/article/download/1510/1300
_version_ 1811200831785009152
spelling Inhibitory processes and cognitive flexibility: evidence for the theory of attentional inertia
Inhibitory processes and cognitive flexibility: evidence for the theory of attentional inertia
The aim of this study was to discriminate the differential contribution of different inhibitory processes -perceptual, cognitive and behavioral inhibition- to switching cost effect associated with alternation cognitive tasks. A correlational design was used. Several experimental paradigms (e.g., Stop signal, visual search, Stemberg´s experimental and Simon paradigm) were adapted and included in a computerized program called TAC (Introzzi & Canet Juric, 2014) to the assessment of the different cognitive processes. The final sample consisted of 45 adults (18-50 years). Perceptual and behavioral inhibition shows moderate and low correlations with attentional cost, cognitive inhibition shows no relation with flexibility and only perceptual inhibition predicts switching costs effects, suggesting that different inhibitory processes contribute differentially to switch cost. This could be interpreted as evidence to Attentional Inertia Theory main argument which postulates that inhibition plays an essential role in the ability to flexibly switch between tasks and/or representations.
Introzzi, Isabel
Canet-Juric, Lorena
Montes, Silvana
López, Soledad
Mascarello, Graziella
Inhibitory process
cognitive flexibility
Attentional Inertia Theory.
8
2
Artículo de revista
Journal article
2015-07-01T00:00:00Z
2015-07-01T00:00:00Z
2015-07-01
application/pdf
Universidad San Buenaventura - USB (Colombia)
International Journal of Psychological Research
2011-2084
2011-7922
https://revistas.usb.edu.co/index.php/IJPR/article/view/1510
10.21500/20112084.1510
https://doi.org/10.21500/20112084.1510
spa
https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/
International Journal of Psychological Research - 2015
61
75
Allport, D. A., Styles, E. A., & Hsieh, S. (1994). Shifting intentional set: Exploring the dynamic control of tasks. In C. Umilta, & M. Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and performance XV (pp. 421-452). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Allport, A., & Wyllie, G. (1999). Task-switching: Positive and negative priming of task-set. In G. W. Humphreys, J. Duncan, & A. M. Treisman (Eds.), Attention, space an action: Studies in cognitive neuroscience (pp. 273-296). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Allport, A., & Wylie, G. (2000). Task switching, stimulus-response bindings, and negative priming. In S. Monsell & J. Driver (Eds.), Control of cognitive processes: Attention and performance XVIII (pp. 35–70). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Anderson, M. C., & Bjork, R. A. (1994). Mechanisms of inhibition in long-term memory: A new taxonomy. In D. Dagenbach & T. Carr (Eds.), Inhibitory processes in attention, memory, and language (pp. 265-325). New York: Academic Press. Altmann, E. M., & Gray, W. D. (2008). An integrated model of cognitive control in task switching. Psychological Review, 115(3), 602-639. Arbuthnott, K. D. (2005). The effect of repeated imagery on memory. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 19(7), 843-866. Band, G. P. H., van der Molen, M., & Logan, G. (2003). Horse-race model simulations of the stop-signal procedure, Acta Psychologica. 112, 105-142. Darowski, E. S., Helder, E., Zacks, R. T., Hasher, L., & Hambrick, D. Z. (2008). Age-Related Differences in Cognition: The Role of Distraction Control. Neuropsychology, 22(5), 638-644. Davidson, M. C., Amso, D., Anderson, L. C., & Diamond, A. (2006). Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4 to 13 years: Evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching. Neuropsychologia, 44, 2037-2078. R E S E A R C H INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH Procesos Inhibitorios y Flexibilidad Cognitiva Introzzi et al. (2015) Int.j.psychol.res. 8 (2) PP. 60 - 74 73 Diamond, A. (2013) Executive Functions. Annual Review of Psychology. 64, 135-168. Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception & Psychophysics, 16, 143-149. Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2004). The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: A latent-variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 133, 101-135. Gandolfi, E., Viterbori, P., Traverso, L., & Usai, M. C. (2014). Inhibitory processes in toddlers: a latent-variable approach. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1–11. Goschke, T. (2000). Decomposing the central executive: Persistence, deactivation, and reconfiguration of voluntary task set. In S. Monsell and J. Driver (Eds.), Control of cognitive processes: Attention and performance XVIII (pp. 331-356). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Introzzi, I. & Canet Juric, L. (2014). Evaluación de las Funciones Ejecutivas. XVIII Congreso Nacional de Psicodiagnóstico. Asociación Argentina de Estudio e Investigación en Psicodiagnóstico ADEIP. Mar del Plata, Argentina. Disponible en www.adeip.org.ar/Congreso2014 Jersild, A.T. (1927). Mental set and shift. Archives of Psychology, 89, 5-82. Joormann, J., & Gotlib, I.H. (2008). Updating the contents of working memory in depression: Interference from irrelevant negative material. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117, 206-213. Kirkham, N. Z., Cruess, L., & Diamond, A. (2003). Helping children apply their knowledge to their behavior on a dimension-switching task. Developmental Science, 6, 449-467. Kirkham, N. Z., & Diamond, A. (2003). Sorting between theories of perseveration: Performance in conflict tasks requires memory, attention, and inhibition. Developmental Science, 6, 474-476. Koch, I., Gade, M., Schuch, S., & Philipp, A. (2010). The role of inhibition in task switching: A review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17(1), 1-14. Logan, G. D., Schachar, R. J., & Tannock, R. (1997). Impulsivity and inhibitory control. Psychological Science, 8, 60-64. Mayr, U., & Keele, S. W. (2000). Changing internal constraints on action: The role of backward inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129, 4-26. Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. (2012). The Nature and Organization of individual differences in Executive Functions: Four General Conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(1), 8-14. Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M., Witzki, A. & Howerter, A. (2000). The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and their contributions to Complex "Frontal Lobe" Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49-100. Monsell, S. (2003). Task switching. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(3), 134-140. Morein-Zamir, S., & Kingstone, A. (2006) Fixation offset and stop signal intensity effects on saccadic countermanding: a crossmodal investigation. Experimental Brain Research, 175, 3453-462. Nigg, J. T. (2000). On inhibition/disinhibition in developmental psychopathology: Views from cognitive and personality psychology and a working inhibition taxonomy. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 220-246. Oberauer, K. (2001). Removing irrelevant information from working memory: Individual and age differences in short-term recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 27, 948–957. Oberauer K. (2002). Access to information in working memory: exploring the focus of attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition, 28, 411–421. Osman, A., Kornblum, S., & Meyer, D.E. (1986). The Point-of-No-Return in Choice Reaction-Time - Controlled and Ballistic Stages of Response Preparation. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 12, 243–258. Osman, A., Kornblum, S., & Meyer, D. E. (1990). Does response programming necessitate response execution? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 16, 183-198. Philipp, A. M., Kalinich, C., Koch, I., & Schubotz, R. I. (2008). Mixing costs and switch costs when switching stimulus dimensions in serial predictions. Psychological Research, 72, 405–414. Schachar, R. J., & Logan, G. D. (1990). Impulsivity and inhibitory control in normal development and childhood psychopathology. Developmental Psychology, 26, 710–720. R E S E A R C H INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH Procesos Inhibitorios y Flexibilidad Cognitiva Introzzi et al. (2015) Int.j.psychol.res. 8 (2) PP. 60 - 74 74 Schachar, R., Tannock, R., Marriott, M., & Logan, G. (1995). Deficient inhibitory control in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 23, 411– 437. Schneider, D. W., & Logan, G. D. (2005). Modeling task switching without switching tasks: A short-term priming account of explicitly cued performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134, 343-367. Simon, J. R., & Rudell, A, P. (1967). Auditory S-R compatibility: The effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. Journal of Applied Psychology, 51,300- 304. Sternberg, S. (1969). Memory-scanning: Mental processes revealed by reaction-time experiments. American Scientist, 57(4), 421-457. Tresiman, A.M., & Gelade, G. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97-136. Treisman, A., & Sato, S. (1990). Conjunction search revisited. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 16, 459-478. Tsal, Y., Shalev, L., & Mevorach, C. (2005). The diversity of attention deficits in ADHD: The prevalence of four cognitive factors in ADHD versus controls. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 38(2), 142-157. Verbruggen, F., & De Houwer, J. (2007). Do emotional stimuli interfere with response inhibition? Evidence from the stop signal paradigm. Cognition & Emotion, 21, 391-40. Verbruggen, F., & Logan, G. D. (2009). Automaticity of cognitive control: Goal priming in response-inhibition paradigms. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35(5), 1381-1388. Verbruggen, F., Logan, G.D., & Stevens, M.A. (2008). STOP--‐IT: Windows executable software for the stop-signal paradigm. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 479-483. Waszak, F., Hommel, B., & Allport, A. (2003). Task-switching and long-term priming: Role of episodic stimulus-task bindings in task-shift costs. Cognitive Psychology, 46, 361-413. Wylie, G., & Allport, A. (2000). Task switching and the measurement of “switch costs. Psychological Research, 63, 212-233 Yeung, N., & Monsell, S. (2003). Switching between tasks of unequal familiarity: The role of stimulus attribute and response-set selection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29(2), 455–469
https://revistas.usb.edu.co/index.php/IJPR/article/download/1510/1300
info:eu-repo/semantics/article
http://purl.org/coar/resource_type/c_6501
info:eu-repo/semantics/publishedVersion
http://purl.org/coar/version/c_970fb48d4fbd8a85
info:eu-repo/semantics/openAccess
http://purl.org/coar/access_right/c_abf2
Text
Publication